Executive Functions and Language Development in Pre-Term and Full-Term Children

  1. Pérez-Pereira, Miguel 2
  2. Peralbo, Manuel 1
  3. Veleiro, Alberto 1
  1. 1 University of la Coruña
  2. 2 Universidade de Santiago de Compostela
    info

    Universidade de Santiago de Compostela

    Santiago de Compostela, España

    ROR https://ror.org/030eybx10

Libro:
Literacy Studies

ISSN: 2214-000X 2214-0018

ISBN: 9783319536453 9783319536460

Año de publicación: 2017

Páginas: 91-112

Tipo: Capítulo de Libro

DOI: 10.1007/978-3-319-53646-0_4 GOOGLE SCHOLAR lock_openAcceso abierto editor

Resumen

Research question. The present study aims to compare one group of preterm (PT) children and another group of full-term children in their executive functions (EFs) and linguistic abilities, and to study if executive functions abilities may predict language development.Background and rationale. The term executive functions include a series of cognitive control processes related to the achievement of goals, such as planning, updating of information in working memory, inhibitory control of inappropriate responses, or shifting flexibility. EFs are considered to be involved in neurodevelopment problems. Extremely and very preterm children were found to show deficits of small to moderate magnitude in certain EFs tasks as compared to full-term children (Aarnoudse-Moens, Duivenvoorden, Weisglas-Kuperus, Van Goudoever, & Oosterlaan, 2012). It remains to check if these deficits also affect low risk preterm children. Relationships between EFs and language development are well known, particularly those concerning working memory. However, children with language difficulties seem to show deficits in other EFs as well.Methods. A sample of low risk PT children originally formed by 151 children and another group of 49 FT children were longitudinally followed from birth to the age of 5 years. Different tasks to assess executive functions were applied to the children when they were 4, and 5 years of age, among them: language working memory, visuospatial working memory, inhibitory control, risk taking strategies, flexibility, and sustained attention. Different dimensions of language development were assessed through different tests at the same ages: language comprehension, phonological development, vocabulary comprehension, morphosyntactic production, comprehension of grammar structures and pragmatics.Results. PT children did not obtain lower results than the FT children in any EFs task, with the exception of more errors committed in the rapid naming task. The group of PT children tended to show lower results than the FT children in all language tests, although significant differences were only found in language comprehension (RDLS) at age 4.Linear regression analyses indicate that EFs, verbal sequential memory and performance in rapid naming task in particular, had a significant effect on phonological development, productive ability in morphophonology, pragmatic development, and grammar understanding.Discussion. The results found indicate that low risk PT children do not show generalized delays in executive functions or language development, in contrast to extremely or very PT children. Executive functions were found to have a moderate predictive effect on the development of several language dimensions, although not so strong on others. Verbal sequential memory, in the first place, attention and inhibitory control seem to have the strongest effect on language, particularly on phonological development, grammar development (comprehension and production) and pragmatic abilities.

Referencias bibliográficas

  • Aarnoudse-Moens, C. S. H., Smidts, D. P., Oosterlaan, J., Duivenvoorden, H. J., & Weisglas-Kuperus, N. (2009). Executive function in very preterm children at early school age. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 37, 981–993.
  • Aarnoudse-Moens, C. S., Duivenvoorden, H. J., Weisglas-Kuperus, N., Van Goudoever, J. B., & Oosterlaan, J. (2012). The profile of executive function in very preterm children at 4 to 12 years. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 54(3), 247–253. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8749.2011.04150.x .
  • Adams, A.-M., & Gathercole, S. E. (1995). Phonological working memory and speech production in young children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 38, 493–414.
  • Adams, A.-M., & Gathercole, S. E. (1996). Phonological working memory and spoken language development in children. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 49A, 216–233.
  • Aguado, G. (1999). Desarrollo de la morfosintaxis en el niño (TSA). Madrid: CEPE.
  • Anderson, P. J., De Luca, C. R., Hutchinson, E., Spencer-Smith, M. M., Roberts, G., Doyle, L. W., & Victorian Infant Collaborative Study Group. (2011). Attention problems in a representative sample of extremely preterm/extremely low birth weight children. Developmental Neuropsychology, 36, 57–73.
  • Archibald, L. M., & Gathercole, S. E. (2007). Nonword repetition in specific language impairment: More than a phonological short-term memory deficit. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14, 919–924.
  • Barkley, R. A. (1997). Behavioral inhibition, sustained attention, and executive functions: Constructing a unifying theory of ADHD. Psychological Bulletin, 121, 65–94.
  • Baron, I. S., Kerns, K. A., Müller, U., Ahronovich, M. D., & Litman, F. R. (2012). Executive functions in extremely low birth weight and late-preterm preschoolers: Effects on working memory and response inhibition. Child Neuropsychology, 18, 586–599.
  • Bayless, S., & Stevenson, J. (2007). Executive functions in school-age children born very prematurely. Early Human Development, 83, 247–254.
  • Beauchamp, M. H., Thompson, D. K., Howard, K., Doyle, L. W., Egan, G. F., Inder, T. E., & Anderson, P. J. (2008). Preterm infant hippocampal volumes correlate with later working memory deficits. Brain, 131, 2986–2994.
  • Best, J. R., & Miller, P. H. (2010). A developmental perspective on executive function. Child Development, 81(6), 1641–1660.
  • Bialystok, E. (2011). Coordination of executive functions in monolingual and bilingual children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 110, 461–468.
  • Bishop, D. (2003). Test for reception of grammar (TROG-2). New York: Pearson.
  • Blain-Brière, B., Bouchard, C., & Bigras, N. (2014). The role of executive functions in the pragmatic skills of children age 4–5. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 240.
  • Blair, C., & Razza, R. (2007). Relating effortful control, executive function, and false belief understanding to emerging math and literacy ability in kindergarten. Child Development, 78, 647–663.
  • Blencowe, H., Cousens, S., Oestergaard, M. Z., Chou, D., Moller, A.-B., Narwal, R., Adler, A., Garcia, C. V., Rohde, S., Say, L., & Lawn, J. E. (2012). National, regional, and worldwide estimates of preterm birth rates in the year 2010 with time trends since 1990 for selected countries: A systematic analysis and implications. Lancet, 379, 2162–2172.
  • Blencowe, H., Cousens, S., Chou, D., Oestergaard, M., Say, L., & Moller, A.-B., et al. (2013). Born too soon: The global epidemiology of 15 million preterm births. Reproductive Health, 10(Suppl 1), S2, 1–16.
  • Böhm, B., Katz-Salamon, M., Institute, K, Smedler, A. C., Lagercrantz, H., & Forssberg, H. (2002). Developmental risks and protective factors for influencing cognitive outcome at 5 1/2 years of age in very-low-birth weight children. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 44, 508–516.
  • Böhm, B., Smedler, A. C., & Forrsberg, H. (2004). Impulse control, working memory and other executive functions in preterm children when starting school. Acta Pediátrica, 93, 1363–1371.
  • Brancal, M., Alcantud, F., Ferrer, A., & Quiroga, M. (2005). E.D.A.F. Evaluación de la discriminación auditiva y fonológica. Madrid: TEA Ediciones.
  • Brumbaugh, J. E., Hodel, A. S., & Thomas, K. M. (2014). The impact of late preterm birth on executive function at preschool age. American Journal of Perinatology, 31, 305–314.
  • Caldwell, B., & Bradley, R. (1984). Home observation for measurement of the environment. Administration manual. (Revised ed.). Little Rock: University of Arkansas at Little Rock.
  • Carlson, S. M., & Meltzoff, A. N. (2008). Bilingual experience and executive functioning in young children. Developmental Science, 11, 282–298.
  • Diamond, A. (2013). Executive functions. Annual review of psychology, 64, 135–168. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143750 .
  • D’Onofrio, B. M., Class, Q. A., Rickert, M. E., Larsson, H., Långström, N., & Lichtenstein, P. (2013). Preterm birth and mortality and morbidity: A population-based quasi-experimental study. JAMA Psychiatry, 70, 1231–1240 (Chicago, Ill.).
  • Dunn, L. M., Dunn, L. M., & Arribas, D. (2005). PEABODY. Test de Vocabulario en Imágenes. Madrid: TEA Ediciones.
  • Duvall, S. W., Erickson, S. J., MacLean, P., & Lowe, J. R. (2015). Perinatal medical variables predict executive function within a sample of preschoolers born very low birth weight. Journal of Child Neurology, 30, 735–740.
  • Espy, K. A., McDiarmid, M. M., Cwik, M. F., Stalets, M., Hamby, A., & Senn, T. E. (2004). The contribution of executive functions to emergent mathematical skills in preschool children. Developmental Neuropsychology, 26, 465–486.
  • Farrell-Pagulayan, K., Busch, R. M., Medina, K. L., Bartok, J. A., & Krikorian, R. (2006). Developmental normative data for the Corsi Block-tapping task. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 28(6), 1043–1052. doi: 10.1080/13803390500350977 .
  • Foster-Cohen, S. H., Friesen, M. D., Champion, P. R., & Woodward, L. J. (2010). High prevalence/low severity language delay in preschool children born very preterm. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 31(8), 658–667.
  • Friedman, N. P., Miyake, A., Young, S. E., DeFries, J. C., Corley, R. P., & Hewitt, J. K. (2008). Individual differences in executive functions are almost entirely genetic in origin. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 137(2), 201–225. http://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.137.2.201 .
  • Gathercole, S. E. (2007). Working memory and language. In M. G. Gaskell (Ed.), The Oxford handbook of psycholinguistics (pp. 757–769). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  • Gathercole, S. E., & Baddeley, A. D. (1990). The role of phonological memory in vocabulary acquisition: A study of young children learning new names. British Journal of Psychology, 81, 439–454.
  • Gathercole, S. E., Service, E, Hitch, G. J., Adams, A. M., & Martin, A. J. (1999). Phonological short-term memory and vocabulary development: Further evidence on the nature of the relationship. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 13, 65–77.
  • Grunau, R. E., Whitfield, M. F., & Fay, T. B. (2004). Psychosocial and academic characteristics of extremely low birth weight (< or =800 g) adolescents who are free of major impairment compared with term-born control subjects. Pediatrics, 114, 725–732.
  • Guarini, A., & Sansavini, A. (2012). Language, executive functions, short-term memory and literacy in preterm children: A longitudinal study. Rivista Di Psicolinguistica Applicata, 12(3), 101–115.
  • Harvey, J. M., O’Callaghan, M. J., & Mohay, H. (1999). Executive function of children with extremely low birth weight: A case control study. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 41, 292–297.
  • Henry, L. A., Messer, D. J., & Nash, G. (2012). Executive functioning in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 53(1), 37–45. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2011.02430.x .
  • Hoff, E. (2006). How social contexts support and shape language development. Developmental Review, 26(1), 55–88. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2005.11.002 .
  • Howard, K., Roberts, G., Lim, J., Lee, K. J., Barre, N., Treyvaud, K., et al. (2011). Biological and environmental factors as predictors of language skills in very preterm children at 5 years of age. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 32(3), 239–249. doi: 10.1097/DBP.0b013e31820b7882 .
  • Im-Bolter, N., Johnson, J., & Pascual-Leone, J. (2006). Processing limitations in children with specific language impairment: The role of executive function. Child development, 77(6), 1822–1841.
  • Jarjour, I. T. (2014). Neurodevelopmental outcome after extreme prematurity: A review of the literature. Pediatric Neurology, 52, 143–152.
  • Johansson, S., & Cnattigius, S. (2010). Epidemiology of preterm birth. In C. Nosarti, R. M. Murray, & M. Hack (Eds.), Neurodevelopmental outcomes of Preterm birth from childhood to adult life (pp. 1–16). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
  • Johnson, S. (2007). Cognitive and behavioural outcomes following very preterm birth. Seminars in Fetal & Neonatal Medicine, 12, 363–373.
  • Kerr, A., & Zelazo, P. D. (2004). Development of “hot” executive function: The children’s gambling task. Brain and Cognition, 55(1), 148–157. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0278-2626(03)00275-6 .
  • Kessels, R. P. C., van Zandvoort, M. J. E., Postma, A., Kappelle, L. J., & de Haan, E. H. F. (2000). The Corsi Block-tapping task: Standardization and normative data. Applied Neuropsychology, 7(4), 252–258. doi: 10.1207/S15324826AN0704_8 .
  • Largo, R. H., Molinari, L., Pinto, L. C., Weber, M., & Duc, G. (1986). Language development of term and preterm children during the 1st 5 years of life. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 28(3), 333–350.
  • Loe, I. M., Luna, B., Bledsoe, I. O., Yeom, K. W., Fritz, B. L., & Feldman, H. M. (2012). Oculomotor assessments of executive function in preterm children. The Journal of Pediatrics, 161, 427–433.
  • Lowe, J., MacLean, P. C., Shaffer, M. L., & Watterberg, K. (2009). Early working memory in children born with extremely low birth weight: Assessed by object permanence. Journal of Child Neurology, 24, 410–415.
  • Luciana, M., Lindeke, L., Georgief, M., Mills, M., & Nelson, C. A. (1999). Neurobehavioural evidence for working memory deficits in school-aged children with histories of prematurity. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 41, 521–533.
  • Luoma, L., Herrgard, E., Martikainen, A., & Ahonen, T. (1998). Speech and language development of children born at <= 32 weeks’ gestation: A 5-year prospective follow-up study. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 40(6), 380–387.
  • Luu, T. M., Ment, L., Allan, W., Schneider, K., & Vohr, B. R. (2011). Executive and memory function in Adolescents born very preterm. Pediatrics, 127, e639–e646.
  • Marcovitch, S., & Zelazo, P. D. (2009). A hierarchical competing systems model of the emergence and early development of executive function. Developmental Science, 12, 1–18.
  • Marlow, N., Hennessy, E. M., Bracewell, M. A., & Wolke, D. (2007). Motor and executive function at 6 years of age after extremely preterm birth. Pediatrics, 120, 793–804.
  • Marton, K., & Schwartz, R. G. (2003). Working memory capacity and language processes in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research, 46(5), 1138–1153. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2003/089) .
  • Marton, K., Kelmenson, L., & Pinkhasova, M. (2007). Inhibition control and working memory capacity in children with SLI. Psychologia, 50(2), 110–121. doi: 10.2117/psysoc.2007.110 .
  • Marton, K., Campanelli, L., Eichorn, N., Scheuer, J., & Yoon, J. (2014). Information processing and proactive interference in children with and without specific language impairment. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research, 57(1), 106–119. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2013/12-0306) .
  • Mendoza, E., Carballo, G., Muñoz, J., & Fresneda, D. (2005). CEG: Test de Comprensión de Estructuras Gramaticales. Madrid: TEA Ediciones.
  • Mikkola, K., Ritari, N., Tommiska, V., Salokorpi, T., Lehtonen, L., Tammela, O., et al. (2005). Neurodevelopmental outcome at 5 years of age of a national cohort of extremely low birth weight infants who were born in 1996-1997. Pediatrics, 116(6), 1391–1400. doi: 10.1542/peds.2005-0171 .
  • Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., & Howerter, A. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41, 49–100.
  • Montgomery, J. W. (2003). Working memory and comprehension in children with specific language impairment: What we know so far. Journal of Communication Disorders, 36, 221–231.
  • Moreno, C. (1992). Escala H.O.M.E. (Home Observation for Measurement of the Enviroment; Caldwell y Bradley, 1984) y C.V.C. Cuestionario de la Vida Cotidiana. Sevilla: University of Sevilla.
  • Mulder, H., Pitchford, N. J., Hagger, M. S., & Marlow, N. (2009). Development of executive function and attention in preterm children: A systematic review. Developmental Neuropsychology, 34, 393–421.
  • Mulder, H., Pitchford, N. J., & Marlow, N. (2011). Processing speed mediates executive function difficulties in very preterm children in middle childhood. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 17, 445–454.
  • Newborg, J., Stock, J. R., & Wnek, L. (1996). Batelle, Inventario de Desarrollo. Madrid: TEA Ediciones.
  • Ni, T. L., Huang, C. C., & Guo, N. W. (2011). Executive function deficit in preschool children born very low birth weight with normal early development. Early Human Development, 87(2), 137–141. doi: 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2010.11.013 .
  • Noort-van der Spek, I., Franken, M., & Weisglas-Kuperus, N. (2012). Language functions in preterm-born children: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Pediatrics, 129(4), 745–754. doi: 10.1542/peds.2011-1728 .
  • Nosarti, C., Rubia, K., Smith, A. B., Frearson, S., Williams, S. C., Rifkin, L., & Murray, R. M. (2006). Altered functional neuroanatomy of response inhibition in adolescent males who were born very preterm. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 48, 265–271.
  • Pennington, B. F., & Ozonoff, S. (1996). Executive functions and developmental psychopathologies. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry Annual Research Review, 37, 51–87.
  • Pérez-Pereira, M., Fernández, P., Gómez-Taibo, M. L., & Resches, M. (2014). Language development of low risk preterm infants up to the age of 30 months. Early Human Development, 90, 649–656. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2014.08.004 .
  • Peterson, E., & Welsh, M. C. (2014). The development of hot and cool executive functions in childhood and adolescence: Are we getting warmer? In S. Goldstein & J. A. Naglieri (Eds.), Handbook of executive functioning (pp. 45–65). New York: Springer.
  • Pizzo, R., Urben, S., Van Der Linden, M., Borradori-Tolsa, C., Freschi, M., Forcada-Guex, M., et al. (2010). Attentional networks efficiency in preterm children. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 16(1), 130–137. doi: 10.1017/s1355617709991032 .
  • Poulin-Dubois, D., Blaye, A., Coutya, J., & Bialystok, E. (2011). The effects of bilingualism on toddlers’ executive functioning. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 108(3), 567–579. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2010.10.009 .
  • Réveillon, M., Urben, S., Barisnikov, K., Borradori Tolsa, C., Hüppi, P. S., & Lazeyras, F. (2013). Functional neuroimaging study of performances on a Go/No-go task in 6- to 7-year-old preterm children: Impact of intrauterine growth restriction. Neuro Image: Clinical, 3, 429–437.
  • Réveillon, M., Borradori Tolsa, C., Monnier, M., Hüppi, P. S., & Barisnikov, K. (2015). Response inhibition difficulties in preterm children aged 9–12 years: Relations with emotion and behavior. Child Neuropsychology, 22(4), 420–422. doi: 10.1080/09297049.2014.994486 .
  • Ritter, B. C., Nelle, M., Perrig, W., Steinlin, M., & Everts, R. (2013). Executive functions of children born very preterm deficit or delay? European Journal of Pediatrics, 172, 473–483.
  • Ritter, B. C., Perrig, W., Steinlin, M., & Everts, R. (2014). Cognitive and behavioral aspects of executive functions in children born very preterm. Child Neuropsychology, 20, 129–144.
  • Roello, M., Ferretti, M. L., Colonnello, V., & Levi, G. (2015). When words lead to solutions: Executive function deficits in preschool children with specific language impairment. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 37, 216–222. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2014.11.017 .
  • Romine, C. B., & Reynolds, C. R. (2005). A model of the development of frontal lobe function: Findings from a meta-analysis. Applied Neuropsychology, 12, 190–201.
  • Rueda, M. R., Fan, J., McCandliss, B. D., Halparin, J. D., Gruber, D. B., Lercari, L. P., et al. (2004). Development of attentional networks in childhood. Neuropsychologia, 42(8), 1029–1040. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2003.12.012 .
  • Saavalainen, P., Luoma, L., Bowler, D., Määttä, S., Kiviniemi, V., Laukkanen, E., et al. (2007). Spatial span in very prematurely born adolescents. Developmental Neuropsychology, 32, 769–785.
  • Sansavini, A., Guarini, A., Alessandroni, R., Faldella, G., Giovanelli, G., & Salvioli, G. (2007). Are early grammatical and phonological working memory abilities affected by preterm birth? Journal of Communication Disorders, 40(3), 239–256. doi: 10.1016/j.jcomdis.2006.06.009 .
  • Sansavini, A., Savini, S., Guarini, A., Broccoli, S., Alessandroni, R., & Faldella, G. (2011). The effect of gestational age on developmental outcomes: A longitudinal study in the first 2 years of life. Child: Care, Health and Development, 37(1), 26–36. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2214.2010.01143.x .
  • Scott, M. N., Taylor, H. G., Fristad, M. A., Klein, N., Espy, K. A., Minich, N., & Hack, M. (2012). Behavior disorders in extremely preterm/extremely low birth weight children in Kindergarten. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 33, 202–213.
  • Singer, B. D., & Bashir, A. S. (1999). What are executive functions and self-regulation and what do they have to do with language-learning disorders? Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 30, 265–273.
  • Taylor, H. G., Hack, M., & Klein, N. K. (1998). Attention deficits in children with <750 gm birth weight. Child Neuropsychology, 4, 21–34.
  • Thorell, L. B., Veleiro, A., Siu, A. F. Y., & Mohammadi, H. (2012). Examining the relation between ratings of executive functioning and academic achievement: Findings from a cross-cultural study. Child Neuropsychology, 19, 630–638.
  • Van Baar, A. L., Vermaas, J., Knots, E., de Kleine, M. J. K., & Soons, P. (2009). Functioning at school age of moderately preterm children born at 32 to 36 weeks’ Gestational age. Pediatrics, 124, 251–257.
  • Van Baar, A. L., de Jong, M., & Verhoeven, M. (2013). Moderate Preterm children born at 32–36 weeks Gestational age around 8 years of age: Differences between children with and without identified developmental and school problems. Preterm Birth. Offer Erez (Ed.), ISBN: 978-953-51-0952-5, InTech, doi:10.5772/54623.
  • Vissers, C., Koolen, S., Hermans, D., Scheper, A., & Knoors, H. (2015). Executive functioning in preschoolers with specific language impairment. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1574. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01574 .
  • Vugs, B., Hendriks, M., Cuperus, J., & Verhoeven, L. (2014). Working memory performance and executive function behaviors in young children with SLI. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 35(1), 62–74. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2013.10.022 .
  • Woodward, L. J., Edgin, J. O., Thompson, D., & Inder, T. E. (2005). Object working memory deficits by early brain injury and development in the preterm infant. Brain, 128, 2578–2587.
  • Woodward, L. J., Clark, C. A. C., Bora, S., & Inder, T. E. (2012). Neonatal White Matter abnormalities an important predictor of neurocognitive outcome for very preterm children. PloS One, 7(12), e51879. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0051879 .